Individuals who have the strongest religious faith tend to have the lowest death anxiety. why?

  • Individuals who have the strongest religious faith tend to have the lowest death anxiety. why?
    Access through your institution

Individuals who have the strongest religious faith tend to have the lowest death anxiety. why?

Volume 39, Issue 3, June 2005, Pages 307-324

Individuals who have the strongest religious faith tend to have the lowest death anxiety. why?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2004.02.005Get rights and content

Religiosity, often operationalized in terms of frequency of church attendance or prayer behavior, is related to higher levels of physical health, mental health, self-esteem, and well-being (Fry, 2000, Holt and Dellmann-Jenkins, 1992, Idler, 1987, Johnson, 1995, Kehn, 1995, Koenig et al., 1988, Koenig et al., 2001, Krause, 1995, Levin and Chatters, 1998, McFadden, 1995, Myers and Diener, 1995, Ochsmann, 1984, Paloutzian and Kirkpatrick, 1995). In most studies, variables such as social support and healthy lifestyles were shown to be partial mediators of relationships between religiosity and health outcomes—both physical health and mental health, including well-being (Argyle, 1999, Argyle and Beit-Hallahmi, 1997, Husaini et al., 1999, Idler, 1987, Koenig et al., 2001). However, such mediators did not explain all of the relationship between religiosity and health and well-being, pointing to the possible importance of other mediators. One such mediator may be religious belief. Links between religious belief and well-being are beginning to be investigated. Preliminary findings suggest surprisingly strong relationships between religious belief and well-being, particularly among Christians, for whom religious beliefs may be especially salient aspects of religious membership (Cohen, 2002, Cohen et al., in press, Cohen et al., 2003).

One way in which religious belief could promote well-being is by reducing fear of death (Aday, 1984–1985, Alvarado et al., 1995, Cicirelli, 1999, Cicirelli, 2001, Cicirelli, 2002, Fry, 2000, Ochsmann, 1984, Richardson et al., 1983, Swanson and Byrd, 1998, Thorson, 2000, Thorson and Powell, 1989, Thorson and Powell, 2000). Fear of death is a strong motivator of human behavior, including cultural affiliation. Terror management theory proposes that reminders of our mortality (which result in “mortality salience”) produce strong anxiety that people are motivated to reduce (Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon, 1989). One typical mortality salience manipulation is for participants to write essays about what they think will happen to them as they die, and about their thoughts and feelings about death. Control participants typically write essays about some other anxiety-producing topic, such as about an upcoming exam or a visit to the dentist. People who have been reminded of their mortality (mortality salience participants) punish a person who has violated their cultural ideals, such as a person guilty of prostitution, more harshly than do people who have not been reminded of their mortality (control participants). Because control participants often write essays about anxiety-producing topics, terror management effects do not seem to be explainable simply by anxiety in general. Terror management results are typically interpreted to relate to a desire to achieve symbolic immortality through one’s culture. Cultures outlive individuals, and so by reassuring themselves that they are good members of their culture, individuals can achieve a sort of symbolic, cultural immortality (Rosenblatt et al., 1989).

We would speculate that even more powerful than a motivation to achieve symbolic immortality would be a motivation to achieve actual immortality. An example (perhaps the only example) of actual immortality lies in religious concepts of the afterlife. Belief in an afterlife could provide an especially effective buffer against fear of death and thereby promote well-being. This is an important research question as death anxiety could pose a significant threat to people’s sense of well-being. This may be particularly true in younger adults, because their fear of death is higher than that of older adults (Cicirelli, 2001, Fortner and Neimeyer, 1999, Templer and Ruff, 1971, Tomer, 2000). Death anxiety has been related to existential well-being and spiritual well-being in a few studies (Chibnall et al., 2002, Heintz and Baruss, 2001). The interpretation of such results is somewhat problematic because of a significant amount of conceptual overlap between death anxiety, spiritual well-being, and existential well-being. Therefore, it is not clear how death anxiety might relate to global well-being. However, to our knowledge, no study has linked religiosity, death anxiety with global life satisfaction.

Studies have been done on the relationship of religious variables and death anxiety. The findings are mixed (see Leming, 1980; Thorson, 1991 for reviews). Belief in the afterlife and in related religious variables has been shown to be negatively correlated with fear of death in some studies (Aday, 1984–1985, Alvarado et al., 1995, Cicirelli, 2001, Cicirelli, 2002, Ochsmann, 1984, Richardson et al., 1983, Swanson and Byrd, 1998, Templer, 1972). However, a study in college students showed that religious variables did not correlate with death anxiety (Templer & Dotson, 1970). In addition, Thorson and Powell (1989) showed a non-significant correlation between a measure of intrinsic religiosity and death anxiety.

Of particular relevance to the terror management literature are studies looking at the relationships of death salience and belief in the afterlife. A few studies have been conducted, again with mixed results. Osarchuk and Tatz (1973) found that increased salience of death was associated with greater reported belief in an afterlife, though Ochsmann (1984) failed to replicate this finding. Peterson and Greil (1990), analyzing national probability sampling data from the 1984 General Social Survey, found that those participants who knew someone who died in the five years prior to being interviewed did have slightly higher levels of feeling close to God, literal belief in the Bible, regular prayer, and church attendance. Whereas controlling for education, income, sex, and age somewhat reduced the magnitude of the correlations, death experience was still associated with slightly higher levels of prayer and church attendance. In contrast, Berman (1974) found little support for the hypothesis that undergoing a life-threatening experience would increase belief in the afterlife. Recently, Dechesne et al. (2003) showed that people encouraged to believe in an afterlife did not show typical mortality salience effects via the terror management paradigm, as compared to people discouraged to believe in the afterlife.

We suggest that part of the reason that the literatures just discussed might be conflicting is because of an under-appreciation of what measures of religiosity would be most appropriate in these studies. In these types of research—and, indeed, in much research in psychology and religion—measures of intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity (Allport & Ross, 1967) are commonly used (e.g., Swanson & Byrd, 1998). The core concept of intrinsic religiosity (IR), as originally proposed, is living one’s religion with sincerity and intentionality. In contrast, the core idea of extrinsic religiosity (ER), as originally proposed, is using religion for instrumental purposes, including for the cultivation of social relationships (Allport & Ross, 1967). IR has clearly and consistently been interpreted as being more normative than ER (Cohen et al., in press, Hunt and King, 1971).

Cohen and colleagues (in press) recently noted that IR appears to be related to private, emotional aspects of religiosity. For example, an item from Allport and Ross’ (1967) IR subscale is “The prayers I say when I am alone carry as much meaning and personal emotion as those said by me during services.” In contrast, community-related or social religious motivations for religion, as well as items tapping ritual or structural practice of religion, load on ER scales; they are thus seen as non-normative. For instance, the following item is from Allport and Ross’ (1967) ER subscale: “A primary reason for my interest in religion is that my church is a congenial social activity.”

Very similar arguments can be made about other commonly used religiosity scales, as well. For instance, the internal and external religiosity scales formulated by Batson, Schoenrade, and Ventis (1993) clearly privilege private motivations for religion over social ones. Batson and colleagues (p. 169) stated in no uncertain terms an assumption that social motivations for religion were non-normative: “We assumed that religion that was a response to social influence would reflect an extrinsic, means orientation because it would presumably be motivated by a desire to gain the self-serving, extrinsic end of social approval.”

Cohen and colleagues (in press) noted that different religious communities see the relative value of private motivations for religiosity and social motivations for religiosity differently. In American Protestantism religions, private and emotional religious motivations are the gold standard (Lindbeck, 1984, MacIntyre, 1984). This may be related to the strong relationship between American Protestantism and individualism (e.g., Bellah et al., 1985, Markus and Kitayama, 1991, Snibbe and Markus, 2003). However, certain religions, such as Judaism and Catholicism are more collectivistic (Lynch, 2000, Sampson, 2000). Such collectivistic religions recognize social motivations and structural practices to be as normative as private, emotional motivations for religion (Cohen et al., in press). Therefore, Cohen and colleagues argued, the set of existing scales that measure IR and ER, including the most commonly used scales (Allport and Ross, 1967, Batson et al., 1993, Hoge, 1972), are most appropriate for use in American Protestant samples. Consequently, the scales may fail to adequately capture a sense of different normative religious motivations in different religions (Cohen et al., in press).

We thus propose that IR and ER scales may be of differential utility among Protestants and Catholics in examining links between religiosity, death anxiety, afterlife belief and life satisfaction. In this study, we investigated the relationships of IR and ER, belief in the afterlife, death anxiety, and life satisfaction among Catholic and Protestant adolescents and young adults. Given Cohen and colleagues (in press) analysis of the differential normative weights of items that load on IR and ER scales among Catholic and Protestants, our hypotheses were that (a) mean differences would be seen in the scales such that Catholics would score higher on ER and lower on IR than Protestants; (b) IR and ER would correlate more strongly with death anxiety and belief in the afterlife in Protestants as opposed to Catholics; (c) that path analytic regression analysis would show more of a path between IR and ER and life satisfaction among Protestants as compared to Catholics; and (d) and that religious affiliation (Catholic versus Protestant) would moderate relationships between IR and ER with death anxiety, belief in the afterlife, and life satisfaction.

Participants were 375 young adults (age range = 13 to 25, M = 18.4, SD = 2.9), 118 males and 257 females. Participants were high school students and youth group members from both New Jersey and Pennsylvania and college students recruited from Philadelphia University. Participants represented a broad range of religious affiliations, but were mostly agnostic, atheistic, Protestant or Roman Catholic. Some, but not all, college level participants received extra credit for participation. Any other

Table 1 has the mean scale scores for Catholics and Protestants. Catholics and Protestants scored similarly on death anxiety, belief in afterlife, and life satisfaction. Protestants were higher in IR, whereas Catholics were higher in ER. Table 1 also shows tests for homogeneity of variance between Catholics and Protestants on the measures. It is interesting to note that the variance for all measures was greater in Protestants than in Catholics, significantly so for ER, and marginally

Levels of death anxiety, belief in afterlife, and life satisfaction were similar in Catholics and Protestants. In Catholic and Protestants, life satisfaction was negatively correlated with death anxiety and positively correlated with belief in the afterlife. Religious differences were seen in that Protestants scored higher on IR than Catholics, whereas Catholics scored higher on ER; also, whereas IR and ER scales correlated negatively among Protestants, they correlated positively, though

  • M.R. Zedek
  • J.S. Levin et al.
  • R.H. Aday
  • K.A. Alvarado et al.
  • G.W. Allport et al.
  • American Psychological Association (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American...
  • M. Argyle
  • M. Argyle et al.
  • D.C. Batson et al.
  • R.N. Bellah et al.

  • A.L. Berman
  • J.T. Chibnall et al.
  • V.G. Cicirelli
  • V.G Cicirelli
  • V.G. Cicirelli
  • A.B. Cohen
  • A.B. Cohen
  • Cohen, A. B., Hall, D. E., Koenig, H. G., & Meador, K. (in press). Social versus individual motivation: Implications...
  • A.B. Cohen et al.
  • A.B. Cohen et al.
  • A.B. Cohen et al.
  • M.M. Coke
  • L.J. Collett et al.
  • M. Dechesne et al.
  • E. Diener et al.
  • E.H. Erikson
  • J.R. Feagin
  • B.V. Fortner et al.
  • P.S. Fry
  • R.L. Gorsuch et al.
  • L.M. Heintz et al.
  • P.C. Hill et al.
  • J.W. Hoelter
  • D.R. Hoge
  • M.K. Holt et al.
  • R.A. Hunt et al.
  • B.A. Husaini et al.
  • E.L. Idler
  • T.R. Johnson
    • A critical function of religion is to manage the potential for terror inherent in living knowing the only certainty in one’s life is the knowledge that it will inevitably end. We first provide an overview of this terror management theory (TMT) account of religion. We discuss how children are socialized into faith in worldviews that allow them to believe they are significant contributors to a meaningful life. We consider how religious worldviews emerged and evolved over human history. We then briefly review empirical support for TMT in general and specifically as an explanation of faith in religions. We cover evidence that religiosity reduces death anxiety, that reminders of mortality increase investment in religion, and that religiosity reduces defensive responses to reminders of mortality. We conclude by considering TMT insights into the constructive and destructive influences of two kinds of religious worldviews.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    • Medical students’ mental health and quality of life (QoL) cause growing concern worldwide, but, to date, few longitudinal studies have followed these students. Our objective was to evaluate the incidence, prevalence, and factors associated with quality of life and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress of Brazilian medical students who were followed for a period of two years. Students’ mental health (DASS-21), QoL (WHOQOL-Bref), and religiousness (DUREL) were evaluated in four different waves(four semesters). A total of 312 (54.2%) medical students responded to all four waves. Medical students demonstrated a high prevalence and incidence of emotional disorders. Almost half of the students presented high levels of depression, anxiety, and stress during the two-year follow-up; approximately one out of five students without symptoms at the baseline were considered new cases and these problems have a cyclical nature. Baseline factors such as depression, anxiety, stress, having low income, being female, being in the early stages of medical training and non-white were associated with poorer mental health and quality of life in the follow-up. Our results show that medical students have a high prevalence and incidence of mental health disorders during their medical training. This data may help educators plan preventive strategies.

    • Religious coping is very common among individuals with psychosis, however its relevance to symptoms and treatment outcomes remains unclear. We conducted a prospective study in a clinical sample of n=47 psychiatric patients with current/past psychosis receiving partial (day) treatment at McLean Hospital. Subjects completed measures of religious involvement, religious coping and suicidality prior to treatment, and we assessed for psychosis, depression, anxiety and psychological well-being over the course of treatment. Negative religious coping (spiritual struggle) was associated with substantially greater frequency and intensity of suicidal ideation, as well as greater depression, anxiety, and less well-being prior to treatment (accounting for 9.0–46.2% of the variance in these variables). Positive religious coping was associated with significantly greater reductions in depression and anxiety, and increases in well-being over the course of treatment (accounting for 13.7–36.0% of the variance in change scores). Effects remained significant after controlling for significant covariates. Negative religious coping appears to be a risk factor for suicidality and affective symptoms among psychotic patients. Positive religious coping is an important resource to this population, and its utilization appears to be associated with better treatment outcomes.

    • Would someone raised without exposure to religious views nonetheless come to believe in the existence of God, an afterlife, and the intentional creation of humans and other animals? Many scholars would answer yes, proposing that universal cognitive biases generate religious ideas anew within each individual mind. Drawing on evidence from developmental psychology, we argue here that the answer is no: children lack spontaneous theistic views and the emergence of religion is crucially dependent on culture.

    • The dual-process model of terror management specifies cognitive and behavioral defenses that minimize individuals’ awareness of their mortality. In response to overt mortality reminders, individuals first exhibit proximal defenses: threat-focused efforts to pseudorationally deny or avoid the problem of death. Subsequently, individuals exhibit distal defenses: efforts to maintain faith in a cultural worldview and gain self-esteem, and ultimately procure a symbolic sense of heroic continuance. At both phases, death-related thoughts impose subconscious pressures on the mind, thereby motivating defensive thought and behavior. Ultimately, distal defenses serve to deactivate unconscious death-related thoughts. This chapter reviews extensive evidence for these processes, as well as recent discoveries regarding the provocation of unconscious death-related thoughts, and the relationship between death thought accessibility and emotion. Various remaining research questions are identified.

    • Although variable-centered analyses predominate the religiosity-health literature, they are limited in that they tend to focus on the (unique) associations between a single facet of religiosity and outcomes. Person-centered analyses allow the identification of distinct subpopulations defined by individuals' full response profiles on facets of religiosity. The present study used latent profile analysis to identify distinct subgroups defined by their scores on the Religious Life Inventory-Revised. Using the Lo–Mendell–Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test, we found that a four-class solution fits optimally in two samples of Christian college students, including questioning (high quest, low intrinsic/extrinsic), intrinsically motivated (high intrinsic), high religiosity (high on all religious orientations), and low religiosity (low on all religious orientations) groups. Across both studies, we found, that the high religiosity, low religiosity and questioning groups reported significantly lower levels of psychological well-being compared to the “Intrinsically Motivated” group. These results corroborate studies suggesting that intrinsic religiosity is a protective factor associated with good psychological well-being among religious students and that personal religious struggles (i.e., quest religiosity) are associated with poorer psychological well-being. Our results point to the utility of person-centered analyses to examine religiosity in unique ways.

    • Terror management theory posits that human awareness of the inevitability of death exerts a profound influence on diverse aspects of human thought, emotion, motivation, and behavior. People manage the potential for anxiety that results from this awareness by maintaining: (1) faith in the absolute validity of their cultural worldviews and (2) self-esteem by living up to the standards of value that are part of their worldviews. In this chapter, we take stock of the past 30 years of research and conceptual development inspired by this theory. After a brief review of evidence supporting the theory's fundamental propositions, we discuss extensions of the theory to shed light on: (1) the psychological mechanisms through which thoughts of death affect subsequent thought and behavior; (2) how the anxiety-buffering systems develop over childhood and beyond; (3) how awareness of death influenced the evolution of mind, culture, morality, and religion; (4) how death concerns lead people to distance from their physical bodies and seek solace in concepts of mind and spirit; and (5) the role of death concerns in maladaptive and pathological behavior. We also consider various criticisms of the theory and alternative conceptualizations that have been proposed. We conclude with a discussion of what we view as the most pressing issues for further research and theory development that have been inspired by the theory's first 30 years.

    View full text